Aesthetics Ethics and History in the Arts of Visual Representation
Introduction
The concept of dazzler is a circuitous topic since artifact, and this is especially true when tracing the cultural trajectory of our relationship with beauty. Western and Eastern artists tend for instance to use unlike perspectives to correspond the visual globe, both in the geometric and in a metaphorical sense. Viewers from different cultures and social groups may take distinct aesthetic experiences to the same visual displays (Palmer et al., 2013). Cultural differences might explicate why beauty is attributed to some things, but not to others (Jacobsen, 2010). Aesthetic processing can only be understood, if it is also seen equally being embedded in cultural contexts and being modulated by social conditions.
Unlike Western painters who since the Renaissance tried to create an exact view of a visual surround, Chinese painters never developed a notion of space as a measurable geometrical entity by developing mathematical rules to organize infinite and create precise spatial relations (Delahaye, 1993). Instead, the Chinese outlook emphasizes a dynamic construction for human relations with the environs, even with the universe, independent of exact physical representations or the proper imitation of objects (Sullivan, 1984; Cameron, 1993). Pictorial perspectives employed in Western and Chinese paintings are, thus, fundamentally unlike. Western painters tried to create an exact view of what they see (or what they believe to see); the geometric perspective was adult to create the illusion of iii-dimensionality by ways of a single-point or convergent perspective (Kubovy, 1986). Information technology should, withal, be pointed out that the key perspective in Western art is already an brainchild (Worringer, 1908), and it is not at all a geometrically right representation of what nosotros meet. Mechanisms of size constancy (Pöppel, 1988) recalibrate the projection of visual stimuli on the retina at the cortical level, and thus distort what is mathematically divers. This neural operation in the early visual pathway (Zhou et al., 2016) serves the purpose to maintain the identity of the perceived object. Thus, the different trajectories of abstraction in the Eastern and Western cultural environments have created unique conceptual frames.
Chinese painters accept employed specific ways of emphasizing spatial information compared to Western painters. Besides a typical organisation of spatial information in a vertical style (i.east., far objects appear in the upper part while shut objects appear in the lower part of a curlicue painting), a near mutual means of suggesting distance was perhaps the use of a perspective, where parallel diagonal lines strike off from the plane of the moving picture. The distinctive characteristics of parallel projections is that lines parallel in fact are as well parallel in the drawing. The angles of these obliques are coherent throughout the plane (Tyler and Chen, 2011). Moreover, Western artists are inclined to capture a specific moment in a visual scene and prepare the physical position of the viewer. In contrast, when looking at a Chinese landscape painting, there is no distinct point to guide viewers. The Chinese outlook has a dynamic quality that integrates successive time windows (Bao et al., 2015), and encompasses a panoramic view of the visual scene, which can be mayhap associated with a floating view (Tyler and Chen, 2011).
Some other concept with respect to differences betwixt Eastern and Western landscapes (Pöppel, 2006) distinguishes on the psychological level betwixt an internal view ("Ich-Nähe" in German) and an external view ("Ich-Ferne" in German language); (information technology should be mentioned in passing that in this area of inquiry many publications are available in other languages that remain mute for the only English language-speaking scientific customs). The cardinal perspective in Western fine art (with its misunderstood geometrical police force) represents an external bespeak of view, and it is characterized by its own aesthetic values; the visual world is expanding in front of the eyes of the viewer (Ich-Ferne). Other than implied by Masuda et al. (2008) who refer to this view as "insider perspective," nosotros characterize this external view as "Ich-Ferne." In Eastern landscapes a completely different psychological machinery is initiated when viewing a picture show from a floating perspective. Because of the multi-layer viewpoints on pinnacle of each other on a scroll class, the spectator has the impression being invited to shift ane's position dynamically, sometimes being located in the air (east.g., looking downward from higher up), sometimes being located on the basis (e.thousand., looking at scenes straight ahead), and sometimes being located at a lower state (e.g., looking upward at faraway mountains); much more than importantly, however, is the psychological consequence of this shifting position that the viewer becomes subjectively a office of the scene. The multi-layer perspectives can exist considered to simulate a three-dimensional space resulting in a virtual circumvolve or ellipse vertical to the picture; inside this imaginary circumvolve or ellipse the viewer becomes part of the scene depicted in forepart of the optics. This implicit construction of subjective space creates the feeling of belongingness or "Ich-Nähe." Thus, we want to submit that the floating perspective does not represent an "outsider perspective" (Masuda et al., 2008).
Another interesting difference with respect to perspective in a more full general sense is related to the pictorial subjects of Western and Chinese paintings. Western artists favor object-centered scenes, whereas Chinese artists adopt context-oriented scenes. Paintings in the Due west typically seek to make the object salient, i.e., to distinguish the object from the background (Masuda et al., 2008). In Cathay it has been otherwise; Chinese artists put cracking emphasis on the context, often with a meditative theme showing minor human figures, as if humans are embedded in a natural environment and awed or inspired by a mountainous mural (Turner, 2009), or even overwhelmed by the sublime (Burke, 1757).
Previous research on culture and aesthetics has demonstrated indeed substantial cultural variations in artistic expressions, such as in drawings, photography, urban center blueprint, product design, or else (for a review, see Masuda et al., 2012). By analyzing the ratio of the horizon drawn to the frame and the number of objects used in 15th to 19th century paintings from Eastward Asian and Western countries, Masuda et al. (2008) provided evidence showing that the East Asian artists placed horizon lines higher than Western artists, and that the size of models in East Asian masterpieces was smaller than that in Western ones. Furthermore, this cultural variation in artistic expressions persisted in landscape drawings of contemporary developed members of North American and Eastward Asian cultures. This pioneer study and subsequent inquiry (Wang et al., 2012; Ishii et al., 2014; Nand et al., 2014; Senzaki et al., 2014) have shown that cultural variations in artistic expressions are empirically testable and robust from a methodological point of view.
Yet, with respect to this methodological point, some other critical factor has to be considered when comparison artifacts from unlike cultures. According to the theory of common constitution betwixt civilisation and the mind (Shweder, 1991; Morling and Lamoreaux, 2008), people should adopt artistic expressions which reflect their ain cultural systems. This prediction is based on the idea that people who are exposed to dissimilar types of cultural artworks could internalize their preference for them. To date, several studies accept documented cultural influences on a broad range of psychological processes, notably attention, motivation, reasoning and self-concept (Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Nisbett et al., 2001; Han and Northoff, 2009).
In spite of the vast noesis already gathered (e.g., Masuda et al., 2008; Ishii et al., 2014; Senzaki et al., 2014), nosotros believe that information technology is still useful to look at one more item when comparing Eastern and Western art, and maybe evaluating the results within a unlike frame of reference. Thus, the nowadays study addresses 1 central question: Are different representations as expressed in typical traditional Chinese and Western paintings appreciated differently by people from unlike cultural groups? To respond this question, we explored the possibility of cultural differences in aesthetic preferences of gimmicky members from the ii cultural groups: Chinese and Westerners. We hypothesized that Western and Chinese subjects would show distinct artful preferences due to the implicit awarding of cultural patterns of creative expression from their own cultures. This hypothesis on "cultural imprinting" is in line with previous observations (Bao et al., 2013b, 2014) in which it was shown that the language environment shapes temporal processing when a tonal and a non-tonal language are compared; this process is suspected to take place on an implicit level by breezy learning (Pöppel and Bao, 2011). It is furthermore suggested that the analytic and holistic strategies are employed also in cognitive processes when representatives from the Eastern and Western cultures evaluate visual artwork validating previous work (e.g., Masuda et al., 2008).
Materials and Methods
Participants
Xl-six university students (23 Chinese and 23 international students from Western countries) participated in the experiment. The Western students were from US, Canada and Europe with fifteen males and eight females. They were aged from 18 to 31 years old with an averaged age of 23.74 years. None of the Western participants had lived in China for more than than 4 years. The Chinese subjects consisted of 9 males and xiv females, aged from xix to 30 years old with an averaged historic period of 23.35 years. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and colour vision, were right-handed, and had no history of neurological disease. None of them were specialists in art history or art theory. Participants were asked before the experiment almost their preference on painting style. They more often than not did non show whatsoever specific interest in a sure painting fashion. All subjects were given informed written consent before the experiment. The study was canonical by the departmental ethical commission of Peking University.
Apparatus
The experiment was conducted in a dimly illuminated room to reduce visual distraction. Picture presentation was controlled by the Due east-prime software organization (Schneider et al., 2002a,b) and displayed on a nineteen-in CRT monitor (1024∗768 resolution, 100 Hz refresh rate). Responses were collected through a keyboard.
Materials
Sixty traditional Chinese paintings and 60 Western classicist paintings were selected from the archives of http://www.artcyclopedia.com and http://www.namoc.org by the authors in consultation with an art specialist. Both Chinese and Western paintings included two categories, namely, landscapes, and people in a scene. The category "landscapes" refers to depictions that treat nature as the main topic, and mainly includes heaven, mountains, rivers, trees, flowers, meadows, houses, and boats. The category "people in a scene" depicts more than one person engaged in activities, coexisting with backgrounds of the land, thus distinguishing information technology from portraits. The paintings were chosen from a diversity of historical periods (from the 9th to the 18th century). We trust to take selected an advisable sample of pictures, but we are enlightened of the fact that some hidden bias may have remained uncontrolled; one has to acknowledge that information technology is impossible to depict in a statistical sense a "true" random sample from artwork, because the population from which to depict the sample is not definable due to the cultural and historical complication. In spite of these constraints we believe to have chosen a off-white sample of typical pictures from the two cultural environments. To come closer to the goal of an advisable comparison, all paintings were low in emotional intensity, that is, they did non describe sexual, ambitious, or religious themes. All paintings were prepared in uncompressed bitmap file format, and the image dimensions varied. Graphic manipulation of stimuli was washed using Photoshop (Adobe). Each combination of cultural style (Chinese vs. Western Painting) × pictorial subject (landscape vs. people in a scene) includes 30 images. Some other forty images (with 10 images in each status) were selected from the same database (from which the images for the main experiment were selected) and used in the exercise session before the principal experiment.
Process
All paintings were presented in random order. Each picture was presented once during the experiment. Afterward viewing each picture subjects were asked to judge its beauty on an 8-Point Scale by pressing ane of viii buttons on a keyboard, where one indicated very ugly and 8 indicated very beautiful. We also recorded reaction time (RT), merely stimulus presentation was cocky-paced and participants were instructed to approach the paintings in a subjective and engaged manner. Before the main experimental trials, subjects were given 10 practice trials under each condition and then they could establish a general impression of the stimuli to exist presented. The images used in the practice trials were non used in the experiment.
Results
The beauty-rating data were subjected to a three-way mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Cultural Style (Chinese vs. Western Painting) and Pictorial Field of study (mural vs. people in a scene) every bit two within-subjects variables and Participant Group (Chinese vs. Westerner) as ane betwixt-subjects variable. The ANOVA revealed a meaning interaction betwixt Participant Grouping and Cultural Style {[F(1,44) = ix.247, p < 0.01, ηp ii = 0.174]}, while both master effects of Participant Group and Cultural Style were not significant {[F(1,44) = ii.597, p = 0.114, ηp 2 = 0.056] and [F(one,44) = 0.010, p = 0.919, ηp 2 = 0.000] respectively}. Further analysis of this interaction displayed interesting dazzler-rating patterns betwixt the two participant groups: for the Chinese group, a significantly higher score was observed for Chinese paintings relative to Western paintings (5.18 vs. 4.72, p < 0.05). For the Westerner grouping, a reversed pattern was observed, i.e., a significantly college score was demonstrated for Western painting as compared to Chinese painting (4.78 vs. four.36, p < 0.05) (Effigy 1). This double dissociation result pattern suggests that Chinese and Western participants prefer paintings that correspond to the background within which they were culturally imprinted.
Figure ane. The significant interaction between Cultural Manner (Chinese vs. Western Painting) and Participant Group (Chinese vs. Westerners) on beauty rating. Chinese and Western participants showed preferences for their own culture's paintings: Chinese participants gave higher aesthetic scores to traditional Chinese paintings than Western paintings, whereas Western participants did the opposite. ∗ p < 0.05.
The ANOVA produced only one significant main result for the Pictorial Subject [F(1,44) = 37.478, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.502]; this factor interacted with Cultural Style [F(1,44) = 19.338, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.305]. For both Chinese and Western paintings, participants gave higher scores to mural than to the category "people in a scene" (Figure 2). Further assay revealed that the difference in scores between Western landscape and figure paintings was significantly larger than that for the Chinese ones (1.20 vs. 0.60, p < 0.001) (Figure 3). No other main effects or two-fashion interaction reached significant level. The 3-way interaction was besides not significant [F(1,44) = 0.549, p = 0.463, ηp 2 = 0.012].
Figure ii. Dazzler rating of paintings as a function of Cultural Style (Chinese vs. Western Painting) and Pictorial Subject area (landscape vs. people in a scene). Both Chinese and Western participants gave higher aesthetic scores to mural than the people in a scene. ∗∗ p < 0.01.
FIGURE 3. The divergence in aesthetic scores (landscape – people in a scene) was significantly larger for Western paintings than that of Chinese paintings. ∗∗ p < 0.01.
Discussion
Enquiry in the past has shown that by using stimuli from the arts, i.due east., from music, poetry or visual arts, one can obtain new insight into cognitive mechanisms which may remain undetected if one focuses merely on simple stimulus configurations as have been employed in the tradition of classical psychophysics (due east.g., from our own research surroundings: Silveira et al., 2012; Avram et al., 2013; Lutz et al., 2013; Pöppel et al., 2013; Zaytseva et al., 2014; Park et al., 2015). With the study reported here, we want to further contribute to this research paradigm past comparing the appreciation of fine art in subjects from the East and the Due west with its challenging differences (Pöppel and Bao, 2016). The present study investigated aesthetic preferences of two cultural groups using pictorial representations from the dissimilar cultures as stimuli. Our results showed that subjects prefer paintings that correspond to their own cultural traditions, i.east., each cultural group evaluated the paintings from their own civilization as more beautiful.
This result at first sight might not at all be surprising equally it might simply reflect the well-known "in-group bias" or "in-grouping favoritism" effect (due east.thou., Tajfel et al., 1971). One could argue that the subjects immediately recognize whether they are confronted with a picture from the East or from the West, and Eastern subjects feel more than familiar with pictures from their cultural background whereas the contrary is true for the Western subjects. If the in-group bias applies in this case, ane has to add, withal, further arguments, which explain the direction of the bias, because such a bias cannot be anticipated with respect to "artful evaluation." In the instance that Eastern subjects would have evaluated Western pictures every bit more than beautiful, and Western subjects would have preferred Eastern pictures (which also could have happened), one would also bargain with in-grouping bias, but with a reversed direction. Thus, information technology is necessary to discover a reason for the direction of the observed bias in our written report. With respect to this question nosotros want to return to ane hypothesis outlined to a higher place that Eastern and Western pictures create a dissimilar psychological state of involvement or "belongingness" (Ich-Nähe vs. Ich-Ferne). It is argued that the pictures trigger a culturally specific feeling of identity (Pöppel, 2010). A Western subject area looking at a Western picture is supported in his feeling of cultural identity, and the same is true for an Eastern field of study when looking at an Eastern picture. We want to submit that the creation and maintenance of identity is one of the most fundamental challenges of the man heed (Zhou et al., 2014), and artwork of one'south own cultures may serve as an important psychological mechanism.
Our analysis may be supported by a recent report in which it was reported that when viewing traditional Chinese landscape paintings, Chinese subjects experienced a greater level of relaxation and heed-wandering, and a lower level of object-oriented assimilation than when viewing Western realistic landscape paintings (Wang et al., 2014). With respect to cultural identity, the study past Masuda et al. (2008) may likewise support our viewpoint; they reported that East Asian subjects were more likely to include great details and background when drawing a scene or taking photographs of a model compared to Western subjects.
Some further points have to exist appreciated: Information technology has been argued that Westerners apply more rational or logical methods to a wide range of intellectual and artistic pursuits, in which a mathematical orientation plays an of import role (Kline, 1964). Western paintings, hence, emphasize the creation of realistic scenes as much as possible. In contrast, Chinese artists place more faith on intuitive and artful cognition about nature (Golas, 2014). This faith is bolstered by considerable reliance on personal feelings and emotions embedded into the image, rather than the details and exact appearance provided by sensory modalities. Members of dissimilar cultural groups are repeatedly exposed to various examples of visual images from their corresponding cultures, and they may implicitly proceeds noesis (Pöppel and Bao, 2011) almost the dominant aesthetic representation of the world; thus, the appreciation of paintings that obey aesthetic principles inside their civilisation is facilitated.
Consistent with Shweder'southward (1991) argument that psychological processes and cultural products represent two sides of the same coin, Morling and Lamoreaux (2008) farther suggested that civilization and the heed are mutually constructed. A given cultural meaning system is internalized past members of the culture, and those who internalize that arrangement brandish habitual ways of thinking and acting. A contempo report by Ishii et al. (2014) showed that European Americans preferred unique colorings and Japanese preferred harmonious colorings, and these preferences were positively associated with cultural values, i.e., uniqueness among European Americans and harmony among Japanese participants. Another study (Wang et al., 2012) found that East Asians were more likely than their European Canadian counterparts to prefer the moderately complex webpage to the uncomplicated portal page, and the results could be explained by the fact that the Western way of thinking is more self-contained and independent, while most East Asians are more holistic and context oriented. These previous findings, combined with the present results, provide supportive evidence that people indeed prefer creative expressions which reflect dominant cultural meaning systems.
A surprising consequence in our study is that both Western and Chinese subjects prefer landscapes compared to the category "people in a scene." This observation suggests that in spite of the cultural frame of aesthetic appreciation as noted above there may exist an overriding principle with respect to the sense of beauty reflecting an anthropological universal (Bao and Pöppel, 2012). Such an overriding principle at a lower perceptual level is for instance observed in color preferences. Komar and Melamid systematically examined the artistic preferences of people in 10 countries, and found that the most preferred painting was an idealized blueish landscape (Wypijewski, 1997). There is indeed evidence that color preferences are universal across cultures (eastward.g., Eysenck, 1941), although later research revealed that both similarities and differences may exist (Taylor et al., 2013). A stiff example, however, for a universal color preference has been made for blue (Saito, 1996; Ou et al., 2004).
From the viewpoint of Darwinian aesthetics (or "evolutionary aesthetics"), it has been suggested that humans may be biologically primed to find detail features more cute, because these features may have been selected for optimal survival, for instance allowing better decisions most when to motility, and where to settle, and what activities to appoint in (Thomhill, 1998; Zaidel, 2010). Even so, evolutionary theorists have been criticized for regarding art merely with respect to adaptive preferences (Plotkin, 2004). Apart from ultimate adaptive valence, we are given no criteria by evolutionary aesthetics theories for explaining why some objects are generally perceived equally aesthetically superior. Here nosotros suggest that the present finding that landscape is aesthetically more appreciated is not only because it signals restfulness or condom, but as well because its restful or safe features carry added emotional significance.
Information technology is worth noting that the deviation betwixt the preferences of landscape and people in a scene was college for Western paintings compared to Chinese paintings. The artful footing of Chinese paintings is deeply afflicted past the philosophy of Chinese Taoist ideas that emphasize the harmonious relationship betwixt human beings and the creation (Law, 2011). In the optics of Chinese artists, natural scenes have the power to suggest the very essence of life to human beings, and in unobtrusive ways, may therefore act as inspirations to virtue. Indeed, in Chinese landscape paintings we tin find tiny human figures, such equally a fisherman on a lonely boat, a man following a mountain path, or a man meditating in a cottage. Here the relationship between man and the natural world is the reverse of the case of Western paintings. Thus, ane possible caption for the smaller difference in the preferences of Chinese paintings is that Chinese landscape paintings are focusing on the natural scenes with human figures embedded, although modest and non very prominent optically, whereas in Western landscape pictures this is rarely the case.
1 important aspect which should not be disregarded is the fact that pictures in both cultures elicit the attending of the viewer. In this case we are confronted with a surprising paradox which mainly applies to Western pictures. With the cardinal perspective in mural paintings a broad area of the environment is represented which in reality would cover the entire visual field. In the picture, however, the visual angle is much smaller beingness limited to the perifoveal region. It has been shown, however, that attentional command is different for the perifoveal region and the periphery of the visual field (Bao and Pöppel, 2007); this eccentricity outcome of attentional control has been well documented with a number of different experimental paradigms (east.m., Lei et al., 2012; Bao et al., 2013a). Given this situation we are confronted with a paradox: What corresponds to the visual surroundings in reality, and triggers the two different attentional systems, is contracted in a picture into a much smaller visual representation. This spatial contraction results in a mismatch betwixt the natural perceptual procedure and its pictorial representation. What should stand for physical reality, does non do it at all. On the basis of this paradoxical situation we submit the hypothesis that such a mismatch by itself leads to an external betoken of view. It enforces "Ich-Ferne" as this bogus perspective does not lucifer reality. The viewer has to bargain with an abstraction in the pictorial representation equally has been pointed out a long time ago past Worringer (1908). Quite the contrary, the floating perspective in Eastern pictures supports "Ich-Nähe," and belongingness or embeddedness as indicated above. These different perspectives in a general sense too correlate with dissimilar cerebral strategies. The more analytical strategy corresponds to the external indicate of view, as the viewer is forced to have a position from the altitude; the more holistic arroyo as has been pointed out previously (Masuda et al., 2008; Senzaki et al., 2014) is typical for the Eastern perspective, and as we desire to submit beingness the consequence of the feeling of belongingness and the validation of personal identity. It is interesting to note that such different cognitive strategies have also been observed on a very bones level in auditory processing (Bao et al., 2013b).
Taken together, our study shows both cultural specifics and anthropological universals. Different perspectives presented in traditional Chinese and Western paintings are appreciated differently past Chinese and Westerners, showing a cultural departure in artful preference. The way that artists represent the visual world in their paintings influences the way that viewers perceive their paintings. We suggest that the cultural difference in artful preference is correlated with cultural and social practices in everyday life. Our aesthetic sense is to some extent modulated past the cultural environment in which we abound upward. At the same time, however, results in this report indicate an overriding principle that independent of the cultural groundwork pictorial representations of landscapes compared to people take a higher aesthetic value.
Writer Contributions
Study conception and blueprint: YB and EP. Conquering of data: QL, YF, and YW. Analysis and discussion of data: QL, TY, and XL. Drafting of manuscript: YB and TY. Critical revision: EP, YB, and QL.
Funding
This work was supported past the National Natural Science Foundation of Red china (Project. 31371018, 91120004, and J1103602), the German Academic Substitution Service (DAAD), and the China Scholarship Council (No. [2014]3026).
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absenteeism of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential disharmonize of involvement.
References
Avram, M., Gutyrchik, East., Bao, Y., Pöppel, E., Reiser, One thousand., and Blautzik, J. (2013). Neurofunctional correlates of artful and moral judgments: equal only not the same. Neurosci. Lett. 534, 128–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.eleven.053
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bao, Y., Fang, Y., Yang, T., Wang, L., Szymaszek, A., and Szelag, Eastward. (2014). Auditory perception of temporal order: a comparing between tonal language speakers with and without non-tonal language experience. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 74, 98–103.
Google Scholar
Bao, Y., Lei, Q., Fang, Y., Tong, Y., Schill, K., Pöppel, East., et al. (2013a). Inhibition of return in the visual field: the eccentricity outcome is independent of cortical magnification. Exp. Psychol. sixty, 425–431. doi: ten.1027/1618-3169/a000215
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bao, Y., and Pöppel, E. (2007). Ii spatially separated attention systems in the visual field: evidence from inhibition of return. Cogn. Proc. 8, 37–44. doi: ten.1007/s10339-006-0151-x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bao, Y., and Pöppel, Eastward. (2012). Anthropological universals and cultural specifics: conceptual and methodological challenges in cultural neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2143-2146 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.008
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Bao, Y., Pöppel, Eastward., Wang, L., Lin, X., Yang, T., Avram, M., et al. (2015). Synchronization as a biological, psychological and social mechanism to create common fourth dimension: a theoretical frame and a unmarried case study. Psych. J. 4, 243–254 doi: 10.1002/pchj.119
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Bao, Y., Szymaszek, A., Wang, 10., Oron, A., Pöppel, Due east., and Szelag, E. (2013b). Temporal order perception of auditory stimuli is selectively modified past tonal and not-tonal language environments. Cognition 129, 579–585. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.019
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cameron, A. S. (1993). Chinese Painting Techniques. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications Courier Corporation.
Google Scholar
Delahaye, H. (1993). "Du peu d'effet de la peinture occidentale en Chine auxXVIIe et XVIIIe siecles." in L'Europe en Chine, eds J. Catherine and H. Delahaye (Paris: De Boccard), 111–135.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1941). A critical and experimental study of color preferences. Am. J. Psychol. 54, 385–394. doi: ten.2307/1417683
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Golas, P. J. (2014). Picturing Engineering science in Mainland china: From Earliest Times to the Nineteenth Century. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Academy Press.
Google Scholar
Han, S., and Northoff, One thousand. (2009). Understanding the self: a cultural neuroscience approach. Prog. Encephalon Res. 178, 203–212. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17814-seven
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ishii, Chiliad., Miyamoto, Y., Dominion, N. O., and Toriyama, R. (2014). Concrete objects every bit vehicles of cultural transmission: maintaining harmony and uniqueness through colored geometric patterns. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. twoscore, 175–188. doi: 10.1177/0146167213508151
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jacobsen, T. (2010). Dazzler and the brain: culture, history and individual differences in artful appreciation. J. Anat. 216, 184–191. doi: x.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01164.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kline, 1000. (1964). Mathematics in Western Civilisation. Oxford: Oxford University Printing.
Google Scholar
Kubovy, G. (1986). The Psychology of Perspective and Renaissance Fine art. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Police, Southward. S. M. (2011). Being in traditional Chinese landscape painting. J. Intercult. Stud. 32, 369–382. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01018
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Lei, Q., Bao, Y., Wang, B., and Gutyrchik, E. (2012). fMRI correlates of inhibition of render in perifoveal and peripheral visual field. Cogn. Proc. thirteen(Suppl. ane), S223-S227. DOI 10.1007/s10339-012-0487-3
Google Scholar
Lutz, A., Nassehi, A., Bao, Y., Pöppel, E., Sztrókay, A., Reiser, M., et al. (2013). Neurocognitive processing of body representations in creative and photographic images. Neuroimage 66, 288–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.067
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1991). Civilization and the self: implications for noesis, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., and Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Civilization and artful preference: comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1260–1275. doi: 10.1177/0146167208320555
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Wang, H., Ito, K., and Senzaki, S. (2012). "Culture and cognition: implications for art, design, and advertisement", in Handbook of Enquiry in International Advertisement, ed. S. Okazaki (Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing), 109–133.
Google Scholar
Morling, B., and Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture exterior the head: a meta-analysis of individualism-collectivism in cultural products. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 199–221. doi: 10.1177/1088868308318260
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nand, K., Masuda, T., Senzaki, South., and Ishii, K. (2014). Examining cultural drifts in artworks through history and evolution: cultural comparisons between Japanese and western mural paintings and drawings. Front end. Psychol. 5:1041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01041
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, Thousand., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Civilization and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291–310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ou, L. C., Luo, Thousand. R., Woodcock, A., and Wright, A. (2004). A written report of colour emotion and color preference. Part I: colour emotions for unmarried colours. Colour Res. Appl. 29, 232–240.
Google Scholar
Palmer, Southward. East., Schloss, K. B., and Sammartino, J. (2013). Visual aesthetics and human preference. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 77–107. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100504
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Park, M., Gutyrchik, Due east., Welker, L., Carl, P., Pöppel, E., Zaytseva, Y., et al. (2015). Sadness is unique: neural processing of emotions in speech prosody in musicians and non-musicians. Front. Hum. Neurosci. viii:1049. doi: ten.3389/fnhum.2014.01049
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (2004). Evolutionary thought in psychology: A cursory history. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publications.
Google Scholar
Pöppel, E. (1988). Size constancy and oculomotor modulation of perifoveal lite-deviation threshold. Naturwissenschaften 75, 463–465. doi: 10.1007/BF00364030
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Pöppel, E. (2006). Der Rahmen. Ein Blick des Gehirns auf unser Ich. (The frame. A view of the brain to our cocky). München: Carl Hanser Verlag.
Pöppel, E. (2010). "Perceptual identity and personal self: neurobiological reflections," in Personality from Biological, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives, eds T. Maruszewski, 1000. Fajkowska and Chiliad. M. Eysenck (Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner Publications), 75-82.
Google Scholar
Pöppel, East., Avram, A., Bao, Y., Graupmann, Five., Gutyrchik, E., Lutz, A., et al. (2013). Sensory processing of art as a unique window into cognitive mechanisms: prove from behavioral experiments and fMRI studies. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 86, ten–17. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.517
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Pöppel, E. and Bao, Y. (2011). "Three modes of noesis as ground for intercultural cognition and communication: a theoretical perspective," in Civilization and Neural Frames of Knowledge and Communication, eds S. Han and E. Pöppel (Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag), 215-231.
Google Scholar
Pöppel, E., and Bao, Y. (2016). "Searching for unasked questions: an unfinished "East-West-exploration", in Cultural Patterns and Neurocognitive Circuits, eds J. W. Vasbinder and B. Gulyas (Singapore: World Scientific), 89-110.
Google Scholar
Saito, 1000. (1996). A comparative study of color preferences in Japan, Communist china and Indonesia, with emphasis on the preference for white. Percept. Mot. Skills 83, 115–128.
Google Scholar
Schneider, Westward., Eschman, A., and Zuccolotto, A. (2002a). E-Prime User's Guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., and Zuccolotto, A. (2002b). Due east-Prime Reference Guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
Google Scholar
Senzaki, S., Masuda, T., and Nand, 1000. (2014). Holistic versus analytic expressions in artworks: cross-cultural differences and similarities in drawings and collages by Canadian and Japanese school-age children. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 45, 1297-1316. doi: ten.1177/0022022114537704
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shweder, R. (1991). "Cultural psychology: what is it?", in Thinking Through Cultures: Expeditions in Cultural Psychology, ed. R. A. Shweder (Cambridge, MA: Harvard Academy Press), 73–110.
Google Scholar
Silveira, Due south., Graupmann, V., Frey, D., Blautzik, J., Meindl, T., Reiser, M., et al. (2012). Matching reality in the arts: cocky-referential neural processing of naturalistic compared to surrealistic images. Perception 41, 569–576. doi: 10.1068/p7191
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. (1984). The Arts of People's republic of china, 5th edn. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Tajfel, H., Billig, 1000. Yard., Bundy R. P., and Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1, 149–178. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420010202
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Taylor, C., Clifford, A., and Franklin, A. (2013). Color preferences are not universal. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 1015–1027. doi: 10.1037/a0030273
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thomhill, R. (1998). "Darwinian aesthetics," in Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, eds C. Crawford and D. L. Krebs (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 543-572.
Turner, Thousand. (2009). Classical Chinese landscape painting and the aesthetic appreciation of nature. J. Aesthet. Educ. 43, 106–121. doi: 10.1353/jae.0.0033
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tyler, C. West., and Chen, C. C. (2011). Chinese perspective as a rational system: relationship to Panofsky's symbolic form. Chin. J. Psychol. 53, 371–391.
Google Scholar
Wang, H., Masuda, T., Ito, One thousand., and Rashid, M. (2012). How much information? East Asian and North American cultural products and data search performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 1539–1551. doi: 10.1177/0146167212455828
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wang, T., Mo, L., Vartanian, O., Deceit, J. Southward., and Cupchik, One thousand. (2014). An investigation of the neural substrates of mind wandering induced by viewing traditional Chinese landscape paintings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1018. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01018
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Worringer, W. (1908). Abstraktion und Einfühlung. (Abstraction and Empathy). München: R.Piper & Co, Verlag.
Wypijewski, J., (ed.) (1997). Painting by Numbers: Komar and Melamid'due south Scientific Guide to Fine art. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Google Scholar
Zaidel, D. W. (2010). Art and brain: insights from neuropsychology, biological science and evolution. J. Anat. 216, 177–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01099.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zaytseva, Y., Gutyrchik, East., Bao, Y., Pöppel, E., Han, S., Northoff, K., et al. (2014). Self processing in the brain: a paradigmatic fMRI case study with a professional singer. Behav. Brain Res. 87, 104–108. doi: ten.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.012
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Zhou, B., Pöppel, E., and Bao, Y. (2014). In the jungle of time: the concept of identity as an escape. ıFront. Psychol. 5:844. doi: x.3389/fpsyg.2014.00844
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zhou, B., Pöppel, E., Wang, 50., Yang, T., Zaytseva, Y., and Bao, Y. (2016). Seeing without knowing: operational principles along the early visual pathway. Psych. J. 5, 145–160. doi: 10.1002/pchj.141
CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar
Source: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01596/full
Post a Comment for "Aesthetics Ethics and History in the Arts of Visual Representation"